Rigor Mortis: How Sloppy Science Creates
Worthless Cures, Crushes Hope, and Wastes Billions
Reviewed by Dr. Panchajanya Paul, MD

Hardcover: 288 pages
Publisher: Basic Books; 1 edition (April 4, 2017)
We frequently come across new, exciting, and ground-breaking research
findings in the media. But, most of the results later turn out to be false.
Of the 7000 known diseases, only about 500 have treatments, many offering
just marginal benefits. The cost of medical treatment is highest in the US.
One of the reason cited by the drug companies are the increased cost of
research needed to bring any new treatment out to the market. Medical
research is a long and an arduous process. The first stage of the research
is done on animals, and based on that data, clinical trials are designed for
human beings. The quality of basic research determines whether the medical
treatment based on them will be effective or not. We frequently read about
innovative treatments about metastatic cancers, Alzheimer's, Amyotrophic
lateral sclerosis, promise for a magic pill for obesity etc. in the media.
But these drugs which show benefit to animals under laboratory conditions
fail to show same success on human in real world. Not only that, some of the
studies when repeated on the animals under same laboratory conditions fail
to repeat the previous results. These are some serious concerns about the
nature of biomedical research raised by Richard Harris in his book Rigor
Mortis. Richard Harris is a famous science journalist, and has covered
science, medicine, and the environment for the past twenty-nine years for
NPR, and the three-time winner of the AAAS Science Journalism Award. The
cover of his book says, "American taxpayers spend $30 billion annually
funding biomedical research, but over half of these studies can't be
replicated due to poor experimental design, improper methods, and sloppy
statistics. Bad science doesn't just hold back medical progress, it can sign
the equivalent of a death sentence for terminal patients."
Scientific truth rests on two fundamental assumptions- objectivity and
reproducibility. The same results should be obtained by two different people
following the same methods, and over time. Richard Harris takes us to the
beginning of the scientific research, he writes, "careful science is new
enterprise. Before the seventeenth century, natural philosophers, as
scientists were then called, often relied on the word of authorities to sort
out truth from fiction. For many hundred years, European intellectuals
assumed that all knowledge already existed, and their job was to interpret
the writings of Greek philosopher Aristotle. Around the time of Galileo,
that edifice started to crack. Natural philosophers dared to conduct
experiments in search for the truth...British philosopher Francis bacon
formalized the scientific method: make a hypothesis, devise a test, gather
data, analyze and rethink, and ultimately draw broader conclusions. This
rubric worked reasonably well when scientists were exploring repeatable
experiments in the realm of physics. But biology is a much tougher subject,
since there are many variables and a great deal of natural variation. It is
harder to see a phenomenon and harder to make sure that personal biases
don't creep in".
Problems in study designs and analysis will produce false results. John
Ioannidis first highlighted this problem in his widely cited paper called
"Why most published research findings are false?". Scientists are smart
people who are committed in their research, but they are also human, and
prone to human follies. There are many causes for bad research. Richard
discusses most them. Sometimes the study result is not relevant even if it
is done accurately. "Improvements won't help in many instances when animals
are poor stand ins for human disease. In the case of stroke...adolescent
male animals often used in the studies may not be a good substitute for an
elderly human having a stroke. Any drug doses for animals may differ
dramatically than those for people. He cautioned that the extent of brain
damage in an animal may not be a good surrogate for human disability or
death." Animal studies can fail in human for several reasons. One example
given in the book is about the wo sets of genetically identical mice, one
group placed at a top cage near the light, and the other at the bottom cage
away from light. Mouse prefer dark places, and thus they become more
stressed and anxious, and more immune suppressed as their cage moves from
the bottom of the rack to the top. Thus, even a small detail like the cage
position of the animal can have a butterfly effect and lead the study
astray. Sometimes researchers cut corners to finish the study early. As many
researchers work on the same topic from across the world, there is an
intense competition to be the first one to publish. The top-rated journals
like Nature, Science, and Cell are more likely to publish groundbreaking and
innovative studies. It thus pays more to be the first, than to be right.
Also, publication in high impact journal is tied with tenure, professorship,
salary, and academic appointments. This produces a lot of pressure and
unhealthy competition among researchers fighting for the shrinking pie of
research funding. Universities make it tougher by recruiting and preferring
researchers who can bring grant money.
Finally, there is the issue of honesty, "... an element of nature that we
develop as a child and never let go off. Our notion of right and fair
doesn't form in a vacuum. People look around and see how other people are
behaving as a cue to their own behavior. If you perceive you have a fair
shot, you're less likely to bend the rules. But if you feel the principles
of distributive justice have been violated, you'll say, Screw it. Everybody
cheat; I am going to cheat too. If scientists believe that they are
themselves being treated unfairly, they themselves are more likely to engage
in less than ideal behavior", writes the author. Biology is complex full of
grey areas. On top of it, the very nature of scientific process, means that
there is no final truth. As new data comes and better theories are
developed, the old ones are discarded. The author quotes McNutt "viewed
through the lens of time, just everything that we write down we'll look back
at and say - that isn't quite right."
In the end, Richard Harris gives numerous anecdotes, personal stories and
interviews with top biomedical researchers. He lays out a plan about how the
problems with biomedical research can be fixed. He says, "reproducibility of
the studies will improve if scientists took simple technical steps, such as
validating the cell lines, running proper controls with their antibody
experiments, choosing adequate sample sizes for their mouse studies,
deciding in advance what hypothesis they are testing and so on". The book
also calls for is a more stringent p-values. It is accepted that if a study
reaches a value of less than 0.05, the results are statistically
significant. But these seems not to be stringent enough and many false
results come out. Richard argues that those who use p-values should aim for
an even stringent results of p value less than 0.005. These tougher
standards will ensure that if the study is run again, the results will be 95
percent likely to be statistically significant. Richard also calls for the
research process to become more transparent. This allows others with a more
rigorous look to find out gaps that were initially missed. One idea is to
reward the good guys and shame the rogues. Some journals have already begun
to give badges to the scientists who are doing the right thing. Some
journals are publishing the list of retracted articles along with the name
of their authors involved in those flawed studies. Another step is to
provide guidelines, templates and checklists to assist scientists working on
animal models. This growing awareness among researchers and journals is a
step in the right direction. In the end, biomedical research is a serious
endeavor. It not only involves billions of dollars, but has the potential to
save millions of lives done in the right way.

Dr. Panchajanya 'Panch' Paul, MD, ABIHM, ABPN, FAPA, is an Emory-trained Child and Adult Psychiatrist. He is certified in Holistic medicine and has authored two books: Stress Rescue and Sleep Coaching. Please call 678-851-3512 or email info@hpsych.org to schedule an appointment with Dr.Paul.